National Taiwan Normal University
教育心理學報  回首頁
Apycom jQuery Menus
562 publication date:2024 / 12 / 30
Children’s Emotion Recognition Skills and Cue Usage with Masked Peers
    Author:Yu-Lun Kuo and Yu-Ju Chou
Research Article

The ability to recognize emotions begins developing in infancy and gradually evolves with age, progressing from


recognizing basic emotions to understanding more diverse and complex emotional states. Emotion recognition is crucial


for young children. It is positively associated with improved social skills and prosocial behaviors and negatively associated


with aggressive, oppositional, and other problematic behaviors. After the emergence of COVID-19 in December 2019, the


disease spread rapidly across the globe. By February 2020, the World Health Organization classified the global risk level


of the pandemic as “very high.” By May 2021, the World Health Organization estimated that the true death toll was 2 to 3


times higher than official reports, reaching at least 6–8 million people. Considering the pandemic’s severity, Taiwan’s Central


Epidemic Command Center mandated mask-wearing in eight key locations and implemented home isolation measures starting


in 2020. The pandemic persisted in Taiwan for approximately 3 years. During this period, preschool children in Taiwan rarely


encountered full facial expressions because mask-wearing became ubiquitous outside the home. In schools, masks were removed


only during meal times; even then, children sat behind partitions. Despite the lifting of restrictions, kindergartens continue to


require children to wear masks for safety. Moreover, parents have their children wear masks when going out. The effect of such


measures on Taiwanese children’s emotion recognition skills remains unclear. In general, young children rely on the facial cues


of others to recognize the other person’s emotions. Thus, relevant studies have focused on the recognition of facial expressions.


However, when facial information is incomplete, children rely on alternative sources, such as language, bodily movements, and


contextual cues, that masks do not obscure.


Child development research should thus address this unprecedented scenario and investigate how children rely on these


nonfacial cues to recognize the emotions of masked individuals. Studies outside of Taiwan have already started to do so.


Although most countries lifted restrictions relatively rapidly, Taiwan maintained the measures for > 3 years. Few studies have


investigated how Taiwanese children recognize emotions with incomplete facial information and how they use multiple cues


to identify expressions. Current research tools for evaluating children’s emotion recognition skills primarily use static images


of strangers’ faces as stimuli, which fail to capture real-world dynamics. In everyday life, children recognize the emotions of


familiar individuals during dynamic interactions by processing facial expressions, body movements, verbal cues (particularly


tone of voice), and contextual cues. Standardized emotion recognition tests often feature prototypical emotional expressions


displayed by professionals following specific instructions, but these expressions do not reflect the nuances of children’s real-life


interactions with peers.


Therefore, the present study explored children’s emotion recognition skills when interacting with masked peers, adapting


research tools to better reflect real-life conditions. Instead of relying on picture cards or strangers’ photos, this study used


videos, rather than static images, of real-life interactions with classmates. This approach facilitated an accurate understanding


of children’s emotion recognition skills and cue usage when interacting with masked peers. The present study included 151 children aged 3–6 years. V ideos of emotional interactions with peers were used to evaluate children’s emotion recognition


accuracy and cue usage. The results revealed marked reductions in children’s ability to recognize emotions, such as sadness,


surprise, fear, and disgust, in masked peers. Thus, children’s emotion recognition skills fell short of expectations; they


frequently confused emotions within the same category and struggled with classifying basic positive and negative emotions.


The analysis of emotional cues indicated that children who accurately recognized emotions used different cues for different


emotions. Peers expressed emotions in various ways, which led to variations in emotional cues. Appropriate cues had to be


adjusted for emotion recognition. Not all emotions could be identified through a single cue. Children who could not recognize


emotions effectively were not only unfamiliar with the definitions of and differences between emotional categories but also


struggled to identify appropriate emotional cues. Mask-wearing further reduced their ability to associate cues with the correct


emotions, causing confusion even in classifying basic positive and negative emotions. These findings underscore a need for


educational interventions to mitigate the effect of mask-wearing on children’s emotion recognition skills. Parents and educators


should actively teach children strategies for recognizing emotions. Mastering the use of emotion recognition cues can help


children accurately recognize the emotions of masked individuals. Thus, children should be taught various strategies and cues


for recognizing emotions in daily life and encouraged to observe and interpret the emotional states of masked individuals.


Understanding peers’ emotions can also enhance children’s emotion recognition skills. Thus, peer interaction opportunities


should be increased and children should be guided to pay attention to their peers’ emotional expressions and feelings. The


development of emotion recognition skills from infancy to early childhood involves progression from a basic to more nuanced


understanding of emotions, which is vital for social competence and behavioral regulation. However, mask-wearing measures


substantially reduced children’s exposure to full facial expressions during the pandemic. This reduction was particularly


prominent in Taiwan, where such measures were strictly enforced for an extended period. This unique context provided a


valuable opportunity for studies to investigate how children adapted to recognizing emotions when the primary cue—facial


expression—was partially obscured. Studies from other countries with relatively short pandemic restrictions have begun to


address these questions. However, the prolonged mask-wearing measures in Taiwan offers a unique opportunity to explore


how children develop emotion recognition skills under sustained conditions of incomplete facial cues. The use of videos of


interactions with classmates, rather than static images of strangers, in this study accurately reflected the real-world challenges


children face in recognizing emotions during social interactions. This approach also helped leverage children’s current


relationships with classmates and knowledge of their classmates’ typical behaviors and expressions. The approach was based


on an acknowledgment that children do not interact with strangers in isolation but rather engage in interactions in dynamic


social environments, where they use a combination of verbal and nonverbal cues to interpret emotions. The findings suggested


that more flexible and adaptive emotion recognition skills can be developed in children by subjecting them to a broader range


of cues beyond facial expressions. Moreover, this study highlighted the importance of contextualizing emotion recognition


within the lived experiences of children. Because a major portion of children’s early developmental years coincided with the


period of mask-wearing measures, understanding how they adapted—or struggled to adapt—to these restrictions is crucial. Such


knowledge can inform targeted interventions and educational strategies for enhancing children’s emotion recognition skills.


Regarding educational recommendations on the basis this study, parents and educators must proactively teach and reinforce


emotion recognition strategies—for example, by guiding children to use alternative cues such as body language, vocal tone, and


situational context to interpret emotions. Providing opportunities for meaningful peer interactions can further enhance children’s


ability to recognize and respond to others’ emotions. Encouraging children to practice these skills in both masked and unmasked


settings can equip them with a comprehensive toolkit for navigating social relationships. In conclusion, this study highlights


the profound effect of prolonged mask-wearing measures on the development of children’s emotion recognition skills. It calls


for collaborative efforts from parents, educators, and researchers to address these challenges and support children in cultivating


robust emotion recognition skills that extend beyond the pandemic. The present study clarifies how children adapt to evolving


social environments and offers practical guidance for fostering their emotional and social well-being. 


下載


關鍵詞: young children, emotion recognition, emotional cue, COVID-19


Seeing Oneself in Others’ Stories: A Study on Audience Experience in Autobiographical Therapeutic Performanc

Copyright © 2025 Bulletin of Educational Psychology
Address: No. 162 Hoping E. Rd. Sec. 1, Taipei 10610, Taiwan, R.O.C.
Tel: (02) 77493757, Fax: (02) 23413865, Email: t05002@ntnu.edu.tw
All rights reserved. BEISU Design